Education Quarterly Reviews
ISSN 2621-5799
Published: 22 March 2023
Learning: An Evolutionary Perspective
André Petitat
University of Lausanne, Switzerland
Download Full-Text Pdf
10.31014/aior.1993.06.01.729
Pages: 563-581
Keywords: Learning, Evolution, Social Learning, Education, Artifact, Algorithm, Information
Abstract
From an evolutionary perspective, this article presents a synthesis of the work on learning and advocates conceptual changes. It distinguishes several levels of learning: genomic, sensorimotor and symbolic. It also suggests a re-examination of the concepts of artifact, algorithm and information, extending them to all living things and differentiating them according to the levels of learning. These modifications are justified by the numerous discoveries that have modified our representations during the last decades. Epigenetics has shaken the relationship between the innate and the acquired, neuroscience has made it possible to go beyond behaviorist dogmas, ethology has brought humans and animals closer together, social learning has emerged as a basic skill leading to languages and cultures, and psychology has shed more light on the roots of the symbolic in the sensorimotor. Each of these different advances in our knowledge has an impact on the conception of learning and invite us to adapt our conceptual tools. Such an adaptation should allow us to better apprehend the digital revolution and its learning modalities.
References
Aguilar, L., Bennati, S. & Helbing, D. (2019). How learning can change the course of evolution. PLoS One, 14(9), e0219502
Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvik, E. & Chittka, L. (2016). Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS Biology, 14(10), e1002564
Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A. & Sheldon, B. C. (2015). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518, 538–541
Ardiel, E. L., Yul, A. L., Giles, A. & Rankin, C. H. (2017). Habituation as an adaptive shift in response strategy mediated by neuropeptides. Nature Partner Journal Science of Learning, 2(9)
Aristotle. (1997). The Nicomachean Ethics. Wordsworth Editions Ltd
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S. & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191-208
Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall
Bates, M. J. (2005). Information and knowledge: An evolutionary framework for information science. Information Research,10(4)
Bates, M. J. (2006). Fundamental forms of information. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 57(8),1033–1045
Baugh, L. & Troy, D. (2020). Nongenetic inheritance and multigenerational plasticity in the nematode C. elegans. ELife, 9, e58498
Beck, B. (1980). Animal Tool Behavior. Garland STPM Press
Beecher, M. D. & Akçay, Ç. (2020. Social factors in bird-song development: Learning to sing with friends and rivals. Learning & Behavior, 49, 137-149
Boesch, C., Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., Arandjelovic, M., Pika, S., Dieguez, P., Ayuk Ayimisin, E., Barciela, A., Coupland, C., Ebot Egbe, V., Eno-Nku,M., Fay, J. M., Fine, D., Hernandez Aguilar, R. A., Hermans, V., Kadam, P., Kambi, M., Llana, M., Maretti, G. & Morgan, D. (2020). Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nature Human Behavior, 4, 910-916
Bolhuis, J. J., Beckers, G. J. L., Huybregts, M. A. C, Berwick, R. C. & Everaert, M. B. H. (2018). Meaningful syntactic structure in songbird vocalizations? PLoS Biology, 16(6), e2005157
Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N. & Berwick, R. C. (2014). How could language have evolved? PLoS Biology, 12(8), e1001934
Borgo, S., Spagnoletti, N., Vieu, L., & Visalberghi, E. (2013). Artifact and artifact categorization: Comparing humans and capuchin monkeys. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4(3), 375-389
Boulenger, V. (2006). Le Langage et l’Action : Dynamique des liens fonctionnels unissant verbes d’action et contrôle moteur [Unpublished PhD thesis]. University of Lyon II
Boussard, A., Delescluse, J., Pérez-Escudero, A. & Dussutour, A. (2019). Memory inception and preservation in slime moulds: The quest for a common mechanism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 374, 20180368
Bridges, A. D. & Chittka, L. (2019). Animal behaviour: Conformity and the beginnings of culture in an insect. Current Biology, 29(5), R167-R169
Brillouin, L. (1956). Science and Information Theory. Academic Press
Burton, N. O., Willis, A., Fisher, K., Braukmann, F., Price, J., Stevens, L., Baugh, L. R., Reinke, A. & Miska, E. A. (2021). Intergenerational adaptations to stress are evolutionarily conserved, stress-specific, and have deleterious trade-offs. Elife, 10, e73425
Candiotti, A., Zuberbühler, K. & Lemasson, A. (2012). Context-related call combinations in female Diana monkeys. Animal Cognition, 15(3), 327-39
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quaterly Review of Biology, 67, 151-174
Chang, L. & Tsao, D. Y. (2017). The code for facial identity in the primate brain. Cell, 169(6), 1013-1028, e14
Chapouthier, G. (2001). L’Homme, ce singe en mosaïque. Odile Jacob
Chomsky, N. (1999). Derivation by Phase, Cambridge MA, MIT, Department of Linguistics
Cizeron, M., Qiu, Z., Koniaris, B., Gokhale, R., Komiyama, N. H., Fransen, E. & Grant, S. G. N. (2020). A brainwide atlas of synapses across the mouse life span. Science, 369(6501), 270-275
Clarke, E., Reichard, U. H. & Zuberbühler, K. (2006). The Syntax and Meaning of Wild Gibbon Songs. PLoS One, 1(1), e73
Clay, Z. & Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS One, 6, e18786
Clayton, D. F. (2019). Learning birdsong by imitation. Science, 366(6461), 33-34
Collier, K., Radford, A. N., Stoll, S., Watson, S. K., Manser, M. B., Bickel, B. & Townsend, S. W. (2020). Dwarf mongoose alarm calls: Investigating a complex non-human animal call. Proceeding of the Royal Society B, 287, 20192514
Creighton, M., Greenberg, D., Reader, S. M. & Mooers, A. (2021). The role of behavioural flexibility in primate diversification. Animal Behaviour, 180(21), 269-290
Crockford, C. & Boesch, C. (2005). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142(4), 397-421
Cuevas Badallo, A. (2019). The question of animal technical capacities. Revista De Humanidades De Valparaíso, 14, 139-170
Danchin, E., Nöbel, S., Pocheville, A., Dagaeff, A.-C., Demay, L., Alphand, M., Ranty-Roby, S., Renssen, L., Van Monier, M., Gazagne, E., Allain, M. & Isabel, G. (2018). Cultural flies: Conformist social learning in fruitflies predicts long-lasting mate-choice traditions. Science, 362(6418), 1025-1030
De Paola, V., Holtmaat, A., Knott, G., Song, S., Wildbrecht, L., Caroni, P. & Svoboda, K. (2006). Cell Type-Specific Structural Plasticity of Axonal Branches and Boutons in the Adult Neocortex. Neuron, 49(6), 861-875
Dessalles, J.-L., Gaucherel, C. & Gouyon, P.-H. (2018). Information, The Hidden Side of Life. Wiley-ISTE
Dexter, J. P., Prabakaran, S. & Gunawardena, J. (2020). A single-cell eukaryote. Current Biology, 30(11), 2205
Ducatez, S., Sol, D., Sayol, F.& Lefebvre, L. (2020). Behavioural plasticity is associated with reduced extinction risk in birds. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 4, 788-793
Engesser, S. & Townsend, S. W. (2019). Combinatoriality in the vocal systems of nonhuman animals. WIREs Cognitive Science, 10(4), e1493
Engesser, S., Holub, J. L., O’Neill, L. G., Russell, A. F. & Townsend, S. W. (2019). Chestnut-crowned babbler calls are composed of meaningless shared building blocks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 116(39), 19579-19584
Engesser, S., Ridley, A. R. & Townsend, S. W. (2016). Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 113(21), 5976-5981
Falótico, T., Proffitt, T., Ottoni, E. B. & Haslam, M. (2019). Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use. Nature Ecology & Evolution,3(7), 1034-1038
Fields, R. D. & Bukalo, O. (2020). Myelin makes memories. Nature Neurosciences, 23, 469–470
Fischer, J. & Hammerschmidt, K. (2019). Towards a new taxonomy of primate vocal production learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, 20190045
Fisher, J. & Hinde, R. A. (1949). The opening of milk bottles by birds. British Birds, 42, 347-357
Fitch, W. T. (2019). Animal cognition and the evolution of human language: why we cannot focus solely on communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, 2019046
Fleig, P., Kramar, M., Wilczek, M. & Alim, K. (2022). Emergence of behaviour in a self-organized living matter network. Elife, 11, e62863
Franks, N. & Richardson, T. (2006). Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature, 439(153)
Fröhlich, M. & Van Schaik, C. P. (2020). Must all signals be evolved? A proposal for a new classification of communicative acts. Cognitive Science, 11(4), e1527
Garland, E. C. & Mcgregor, P. K. (2020). Cultural transmission, evolution, and revolution in vocal displays: Insights from bird and whale song. Frontiers of Psychology, 11, 544929
Garland, E. C., Gedamke, J., Rekdahl, M. L., Noad, M. J., Garrigue, C. & Gales, N. (2013). Humpback whale song on the Southern Ocean feeding grounds: Implications for cultural transmission. PLoS One, 8(11), e79422
Garland, E. C., Rendell, L., Lamoni, L., Poole, M. M. & Noad, M. J. (2017). Song hybridization events during revolutionary song change provide insights into cultural transmission in humpback whales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 114(30), 7822-7829
Genzel, D., Desai, J., Paras, E. & Yartsev, M. M. (2019. Long-term and persistent vocal plasticity in adult bats. Nature Communication, 10(1), 3372
Gershman, S. J., Balbi, P. E., Gallistel, C. R. & Gunawardena, J. (2021). Reconsidering the evidence for learning in single cells. Elife, 10, e61907
Gibson, D. G., Glass, J. I., Lartigue, C., Noskov, V. N., Chuang, R.-Y., Algire, M. A., Benders, G. A., Montague, M. G., Ma, L., Moodie, M. M., Merryman, C., Vashee, S., Krishnakumar, R., Assad-Garcia, N., Andrews-Pfannkoch, C., Denisova, E. A., Young, L., Qi, Z.-Q., Segall-Shapiro, T., Calvey, C. H., Pamar, P. P., Hutchison, C. A., Smith, H. O. & Venter, J.-C. (2010). Creation of a bacterial cell controlled by a chemically synthesized genome. Science, 329(5987), 52-56
Gould, J. L. (2007). Animal artifacts. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence, Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation(pp. 249-266). Oxford University Press
Gourgou, E., Adiga, K., Goettemoeller, A., Chen, C. & Hsu, A.-L. (2021). Caenorhabditis elegans learning in a structured maze is a multisensory behavior. iSCIENCE, 24(4), 102284
Graham, K. E., Wilke, C., Lahiff, N. J. & Slocombe, K. E. (2019). Scratching beneath the surface: Intentionality in great ape signal production. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1789), 20180403
Grice, P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377-388
Griesser, M., Wheatcroft, D. & Suzuki T. (2018). From bird calls to human language: Exploring the evolutionary drivers of compositional syntax. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 6-12
Gupta, A., Savarese, S., Ganguli, S. & Li F.-F. (2021). Embodied intelligence via learning and evolution. Nature Communication, 12(5721)
Hage, S. R., Gavrilov, N. & Nieder A. (2013). Cognitive control of distinct vocalizations in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Cognition and Neuroscience, 25(10), 1692-1701
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., Boes, X., Quinn, R. L., Brenet, M., Arroyo, A., Taylor, N., Clément, S., Daver, G., Brugal, J.-P., Leakey, L., Mortlock, R. A., Wright, J.D., Lokorodi, S., Kirwa, C., Kent, D. V. & Roche H. (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521, 310–315
Hart, M. P. (2019). Stress-Induced Neuron Remodeling Reveals Differential Interplay Between Neurexin and Environmental Factors in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics, 213(4), 1415-1430
Heams, T. (2019). Infravies. Le vivant sans frontières. Seuil
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. A Neuropsychological Theory. Wiley
Heesen, R., Bangerter, A., Zuberbühler, K., Rossano, F., Iglesias, K., Guéry, J. P. & Genty, E. (2020). Bonobos engage in joint commitment. Science Advances, 6(51), eabd1306
Henneberger, C., Bard, L., Panatier, A., Reynold, J. P., Kopach, O., Medvedey, J. I., Minge, D., Herde, M. K., Anders, S., Kraev, I., Heller, J. P., Rama, S., Zeng, K., Jensen, T. P., Sanchez-Romero, I., Jackson, C. J., Janovjak, H., Otterson, O. P. & Rusakov, D. A. (2020). LTP Induction Boosts Glutamate Spillover by Driving Withdrawal of Perisynaptic Astroglia. Neuron, 108(5), 919-936.e11
Heyes, C. (2020). Culture. Current Biology, 30, R1233–R1255
Hoppitt, W. J. & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods and Models. Princeton University Press
Hoppitt, W. J., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M. M. & Laland, K. N. (2008). Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 23(9), 486-493
Hosken, D. J., Hunt, J. & Wedell, N. (2019). Genes and Behaviour. Beyond Nature‐Nurture. Wiley.
Irwin, D., Bensch, S. & Price, T. (2001). Speciation in a ring. Nature, 409, 333–337
Kabadayi, C. & Osvath, M. (2017). Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science, 357(6347), 202-204
Kandel, E. R. (2001). The molecular biology of memory storage: A dialogue between genes and synapses. Science, 294(5544),1030-1038
Kepler, L. D., McDiarmid, T. A. & Rankin, C. H. (2020). Habituation in high-throughput genetic model organisms as a tool to investigate the mechanisms of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 171, 107208
Kimura, M. (1968). Evolutionary rate at the molecular level. Nature, 217, 624-628
King, S. L. & Janik, V. M. (2013). Bottlenose dolphins can use learned vocal labels to address each other. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(32), 13216-13221
Lachlan, R. F., Ratmann, O. & Nowicki, S. (2018). Cultural conformity generates extremely stable traditions in bird song. Nature Communications, 9, 2417
Lafontaine, C. (2004). L’Empire cybernétique. Seuil
Lafontaine, C. (2021). Bio-objets. Les nouvelles frontières du vivant. Seuil
Laland, K. N. & Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 21(10), 542-547
Laland, K. N., Atton, N. & Webster, M. M. (2011). From fish to fashion: Experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1567), 958–968
Le Calvez, R., Peperkamp, S. & Dupoux, E. (2007). Apprentissage bottom-up des phonèmes : Une étude computationnelle. Mathematics and Social Sciences, 180(4), 101-113
Lechermeier, G. (2019). Le Vivant. La singularité et l’universel. Ed. Matériologiques
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1943). Évolution et technique, t.1 L’homme et la matière. Albin Michel
Li, Y., Li, L., Wu, J., Zhu, Z., Feng, X., Qin, L., Zhu, Y., Sun, L., Liu, Y., Qiu, Z., Duan, S. & Yu, Y.-Q. (2020). Activation of astrocytes in hippocampus decreases fear memory through adenosine A1 receptors. Elife, 9, e57155
Logue, D. M. & Leca, J.-B. (2020). Animal culture: How a new birdsong went viral. Current Biology, 30(16), R9 R957-R959
Marler, P. & Tamura, M. (1962). Song ‘dialects’ in three populations of white-crowned sparrows. The Condor, 64(5), 368-377
Mathevon, N. (2021). Les Animaux parlent, sachons les écouter. HumenSciences
McDiarmid, T. A., Belmadani, M., Liang, J., Meili, F., Mathews, E. A., Mullen, G. P., Hendi, A., Wong, W. R., Rand, J. B., Mizumoto, K., Haas, K., Pavlidis, P. & Rankin, C. H. (2020). Systematic phenomics analysis of autism-associated genes reveals parallel networks underlying reversible impairments in habituation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 117(1), 656-667
McDiarmid, T. A., Yu, A. J. & Rankin, C. H. (2019). Habituation is more than learning to ignore: Multiple mechanisms serve to facilitate shifts in behavioral strategy. BioEssays, 41(9), 1900077
Mesoudi, A. & Thornton, A. (2018). What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceeding of the Royal Society B., 285(1880), 20180712
Monod, J. (1972). Chance and Necessity. Vintage Books
Moser, M. B. & Moser, E. (2016). Le GPS de notre cerveau. Pour la Science, 463, 26-34
Musser, J. M., Schippers, K. J., Nickel, M., Mizzon, G., Kohn, A.B., Pape, C., Ronchi, P., Papadopoulos, N., Tarashansky, A. J., Hammel, J. U., Wolf, F., Liang, C., Hernández-Plaza, A., Cantalapiedra, C. P., Achim, K., Schieber, N. L., Pan, L., Ruperti, F., Francis, W. R., Vargas, S., Kling, S., Renkert, M., Polikarpov, M., Bourenkov, G., Feuda, R., Gaspar, I., Burkhardt, P., Wang, B., Bork, P., Beck, M., Schneider, T. R., Kreshuk, A., Wörheide, G., Huerta-Cepas. J., Schwab, Y., Moroz, L. L. & Arendt, D. (2021). Profiling cellular diversity in sponges informs animal cell type and nervous system evolution. Science, 374(6568), 717-723
Nieder, A. & Mooney, R. (2019). The neurobiology of innate, volitional and learned vocalizations in mammals and birds. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1789), 20190054
Noad, M., Cato, D., Bryden, M., Jenner, M. & Jenner, K. C. S. (2000). Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature, 408, 537
Nolfi, S. & Floreano, D. (1999). Learning and evolution. Autonomous Robots, 7, 89-113
O’Connor, T. & Wong, H. Y. (2015). Emergent properties. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy
Otter, K. A., McKenna, A., Lazerte, S. E. & Ramsay, S. M. (2020). Continent-wide shifts in song dialects of white-throated sparrows. Current Biology, 30(16), 3231-3235
Osiurak, F., Crétel, C., Uomini, N., Bryche, C., Lesourd, M. & Reynaud, M. (2021). On the neurocognitive co-evolution of tool behavior and language: Insights from the massive redeployment framework. Topics in Cognitive Science, 13, 684–707
Ouattara, K., Lemasson, A. & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(51), 22026–22031
Pennartz, C. M., Farisco, M. & Evers, K. (2019). Indicators and criteria of consciousness in animals and intelligent machines: An inside-out approach. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13(25)
Petitat, A. (2020). Towards a trans-epistemic society. Technium Social Science Journal, 4, 120-133
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. Basic Books
Piaget, J. (1978). Behavior and Evolution. Random House
Podos, J. & Warren, P. S. (2007). The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 37, 403-458
Preston, B. (1998). Cognition and tool use. Mind and language, 13(4), 513-547
Proffitt, T., Luncz, L. V., Falotico, T., Ottoni, E. B., De La Torre, I. & Haslam, M. (2016). Wild monkeys flake stone tools. Nature, 539, 85-88
Proffitt, T., Reeves, J. S., Braun D. R., Malaivijitnond S. & Luncz L. V. (2023). Wild macaques challenge the origin of intentional tool production. Science Advances, 9(10)
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1894). Les nouvelles idées sur la structure du système nerveux : Chez l'homme et chez les vertébrés. C. Reinwald & Cie
Randlett, O., Haesemeyer, M., Forkin, G., Shoenhard, H., Schier, A. F., Engert, F. & Granato, M. (2019). Distributed plasticity drives visual habituation learning in larval zebrafish. Current Biology, 29(8), 1337-1345.e4
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago University Press
Rochat, P. (2004). The Infant's World. Harvard University Presse
Rose, J. K., Sangha, S., Rai, S., Norman, K. R. & Rankin, C. H. 2005. Decreased sensory stimulation reduces behavioral responding, retards development, and alters neuronal connectivity in Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(31), 7159-7168
Rudenko, G. (2019). Neurexins - versatile molecular platforms in the synaptic cleft. Current Opinion in Structural Biology, 54,112-121
Scholl, B. & Fitzpatrick, D. (2020). Cortical synaptic architecture supports flexible sensory computations. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 64, 41-45
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(3), 379-423
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(4), 623-666
Shepherd, G. M., Marenco, L., Hines, M. L., Migliore, M., McDougal, R. A., Carnevale, N. T., Newton, A. J. H., Surles-Zeigler, M. & Ascoli, G. A. (2019). Neuron names: A gene- and property-based name format, with special reference to cortical neurons. Frontiers of Neuroanatomy, 13, 25
Sperber, D. (2007). Seedless grapes: Nature and culture. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds), Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (pp. 124-137). Oxford University Press,
Spillmann, B., Dunkel, L. P., Van Noordwijk, M. A., Amda, R. N. A., Lameira, A. R., Wich, S.A. & Van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Acoustic properties of long calls given by flanged male orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) reflect both individual identity and context. Ethology, 116(5), 385-395
Spitzer, N. C. (2017). Neurotransmitter switching in the developing and adult brain. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 25(40), 1-19.
Stefanini, F., Kushnir, L., Jimenez, J. C., Jennings, J. H., Woods, N. I., Stuber, G. D., Kheirbek, M. A., Hen, R. & Fusi, S. (2020). A distributed neural code in the dentate gyrus and in CA1. Neuron, 107(4), 703-716, e4
Stout, D. & Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B,367(1585), 75–87.
Südhof, T. C. (2018). Towards an Understanding of Synapse Formation. Neuron, 24(100), 2, 276-293.
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D. & Griesser, M. (2016). Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nature Communication, 7, 10986
Suzuki, T. N. & Zuberbühler, K. (2019). Animal syntax. Current Biology, 29(14), R669-R671
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D. & Griesser, M. (2017). Wild birds use an ordering rule to decode novel call sequences. Current Biology, 27(15), 2331-2336.e3
Svensson, E., Apergis-Schoute, J., Burnstock, G., Nusbaum, M. P., Parker, D. & Schiöth, H. B. (2019). General principles of neuronal co-transmission: Insights from multiple model systems. Frontiers Neural Circuits, 12, 117
Taylor, H. (2021). Evidence for teaching in an australian songbird. Frontiers of Psychology, 12, 593532
Taylor, W. W., Imhoff, B. R., Sathi, Z. S., Liu, W. Y., Garza, K. M. & Dias, B. G. (2021). Contributions of glucocorticoid receptors in cortical astrocytes to memory recall. Learning and Memory, 28(4), 126-133
Ten Cate, C. (2018). The comparative study of grammar learning mechanisms: Birds as models. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 13-18
Thibault, S., Py, R., Gervasi, A. M., Salemme, R., Koun, E., Lövden, M., Boulenger, V., Roy, A. C. & Brozzoli, C. (2021). Tool use and language share syntactic processes and neural patterns in the basal ganglia. Science, 374(6569), eabe0874
Thornton, A. & Clutton-Brock, T. (2011). Social learning and the development of individual and group behaviour in mammal societies. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1567), 978–987
Thorpe, W. H. (1951). The learning abilities of birds. Ibis, 93(2), 252-296
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. MIT press
Townsend, S. W., Engesser, S., Stoll, S., Zuberbühler, K. & Bickel, B. (2018). Compositionality in animals and humans. PLoS Biology, 16(8), e2006425
Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M., Braga Goncalves, I., Burkart, J. M., Flower, T., Gaunet, F., Glock, H. J., Gruber, T., Dawam, J., Liebal, K., Linke, A., Miklósi, Á., Moore, R., Van Schaik, C. P., Stoll, S., Vail, A., Waller, B. M., Wild, M., Zuberbühler, K. & Manser, M. B. (2017). Exorcising Grice’s ghost: An empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 92(3), 1427-1433
Trigo, F. F. (2019). Antidromic analog signaling. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 13, 354
Tyack, P. L. (2019). A taxonomy for vocal learning. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B, 375, 20180406.
Van de Ville, D., Farouj, Y., Preti, M. G., Liégeois, R. & Amico, E. (2021). When makes you unique: Temporality of the human brain fingerprint. Science Advances, 7(42), eabj0751
Van Schaik, C. P.(2012). Animal culture: Chimpanzee conformity? Current Biology, 22(10), R402-404
Venter, J.-C. (2013). Life at the Speed of Light: From the Double Helix to the Dawn of Digital Life. Viking Books
Walsh, S. L., Townsend, S. W., Morgan, K. & Ridley, A. R. (2019). Investigating the potential for call combinations in a lifelong vocal learner. Ethology,125(6), 362-368
Watson, J. D. & Crick, F. H. C. (1953). Molecular structure of nucleic acids. A structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid. Nature, 171, 737-738
Weitekamp, C.A. & Keller, L. (2019). Genes and behaviour. In D. J. Hosken, J. Hunt & N. Wedell (Eds), Genes and behaviour: Beyond nature-nurture (pp. 93-109). Wiley
Whitehead, H. & Rendell, L. (2015). The Cultural Lives of Whales and Dolphins. Chicago University Press
Whitehead, H., Laland, K. N., Rendell, L., Thorogood, R. & Whiten, A. (2019). The reach of gene–culture coevolution in animals. Nature Communications, 10(2405)
Whiten, A. (2012). Social learning, traditions and culture. In J. C. Mitani, J. Call, P. M. Kappeler, R. A. Palombit & J. B. Silk (Eds), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 681-699). Chicago University Press
Whiten, A. (2021). The burgeoning reach of animal culture. Science, 372(6537), eabe6514
Whiten, A., Ayala, F. J., Feldman, M. W. & Laland, K. N. (2017). The extension of biology through culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 114(30), 7775-7781
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., Mcgrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682-685
Whiten, A., Horner, V. & De Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437(7059), 737-740
Yamagata, M., Yan, W. & Sanes, J. R. (2021). A cell atlas of the chick retina based on single-cell transcriptomics. Elife, 10, e63907
Zuberbühler, A. K. (2012). Call combinations in monkeys: compositional or idiomatic expressions? Brain and Language, 120(3), 303-309